“How will you construct a pathway system?”: Microanalysis of teacher-child scientific conversations


Abstract views: 537 / PDF downloads: 277

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.37291/2717638X.202123117

Keywords:

Teacher-child conversations, Questions, Explanations, STEM, Inquiry-based learning

Abstract

During the preschool years, children’s question-explanation exchanges with teachers serve as a powerful mechanism for their early STEM knowledge acquisition. Utilizing naturalistic longitudinal classroom data, we examined how such conversations in an inquiry-based preschool classroom change during an extended scientific inquiry unit. We were particularly interested in information-seeking questions (causal, e.g. “How will you construct a pathway?”; fact-based, e.g., “Where’s the marble?”). Videos (n = 18; 14 hours) were collected during a three-week inquiry unit on forces and motion and transcribed in CLAN-CHILDES software at the utterance level. Utterances were coded for delivery (question vs. statement) and content (e.g., fact-based, causal). Although teachers ask more questions than children, we found a significant increase in information-seeking questions during Weeks 2 and 3. We explored the content of information-seeking questions and found that the majority of these questions were asked by teachers, and focused on facts. However, the timing of fact-based and causal questions varied. Whereas more causal questions occurred in earlier weeks, more fact-based questions were asked towards the end of the inquiry. These findings provide insight into how children’s and teacher’s questions develop during an inquiry, informing our understanding of early science learning. Even in an inquiry-learning environment, teachers guide interactions, asking questions to support children’s learning. Children’s information-seeking questions increase during certain weeks, suggesting that providing opportunities to ask questions may allow children to be more active in constructing knowledge. Such findings are important for considering how science questions are naturally embedded in an inquiry-based learning classroom.

References

Anderson, R. D. (2002). Reforming science teaching: What research says about inquiry. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 13(1), 1–12. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015171124982

Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., & Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver-child interactions in a children’s museum. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 502–515. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017822

Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322–330. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001

Bova, A., & Arcidiacono, F. (2013). Investigating children’s Why-questions: A study comparing argumentative and explanatory function. Discourse Studies, 15(6), 713–734. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445613490013

Butler, L. P. (2020). The Empirical child? A Framework for investigating the development of scientific habits of mind. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 34–40. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12354

Butler, L. P., Ronfard, S., & Corriveau, K. H. (Eds.). (2020). The questioning child: Insights from psychology and education (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108553803.001

Callanan, M. A., & Jipson, J. L. (2001). Explanatory conversations and young children’s developing scientific literacy. In Designing for science: Implications from everyday, classroom, and professional settings (pp. 21-49). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Callanan, M. A., Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., Jaeger, G. J., Letourneau, S., McHugh, S. R., Willard, A., Brinkman, A., Finiasz, Z., Rubio, E., Barnett, A., Gose, R., Martin, J. L., Meisner, R., & Watson, J. (2020). Exploration, explanation, and parent–child interaction in museums. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 85(1), 7–137. Callanan, M.A. & Oakes, L.M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7(2), 213-233. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12412

Callanan, M., Shrager, J., & Moore, J. L. (1995). Parent-child collaborative explanations: Methods of identification and analysis. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(1), 105–129. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327809jls0401_3

Chandler-Campbell, I. L., Leech, K. A., & Corriveau, K. H. (2020). Investigating science together: Inquiry-based training promotes scientific conversations in parent-child Interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1934, 1-12. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01934

Chouinard, M. M. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72(1), vii–ix,1–112; discussion 113–26.

Crowley, K., Callanan, M., Jipson, J. L., Galco, J., Topping, K., & Shrager, J. (2001). Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent—child activity. Science Education, 85(6), 712–732. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.1035

Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12(3), 258–261. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347

Dean Jr., D., & Kuhn, D. (2007). Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Science Education, 91(3), 384–397. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20194

Edson, M. T. (2013). Starting with science: Strategies for introducing young children to inquiry. Stenhouse Publishers.

Engel, S. (2011). Children's need to know: Curiosity in schools. Harvard educational review, 81(4), 625-645. DOI: https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.81.4.h054131316473115

Fender, J. G., & Crowley, K. (2007). How parent explanation changes what children learn from everyday scientific thinking. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 189–210. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2007.02.007

Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Young children prefer and remember satisfying explanations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(5), 718–736. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2015.1098649

Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Development, 80(6), 1592–1611. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01356.x

Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2016). Becoming brilliant: What science tells us about raising successful children. American Psychological Association Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/14917-000

Greif, M. L., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Keil, F. C., & Gutierrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Psychological Science, 17(6), 455–459. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01727.x

Gutwill, J. P., & Allen, S. (2010). Group Inquiry at Science Museum Exhibits: Getting Visitors to Ask Juicy Questions. Exploratorium.

Haber, A.S. & Corriveau, K.H. (2021). “Why is the stem green?”: Investigating how teacher-child scientific conversations shape children’s learning in an inquiry-based preschool classroom. [Manuscript in preparation]. Boston University Wheelock College of Education & Human Development.

Haber, A. S., Leech, K. A., Benton, D. T., Dashoush, N., & Corriveau, K. H. (2021). Questions and explanations in the classroom: Examining variation in early childhood teachers’ responses to children’s scientific questions. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 57, 121–132. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.05.008

Haber, A., Sobel, D., & Weisberg, D. (2019). Fostering children’s reasoning about disagreements through an inquiry-based curriculum. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(4), 592–610. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1639713

Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child Development, 4(1), 62–67. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00119.x

Haden, C. A., Jant, E. A., Hoffman, P. C., Marcus, M., Geddes, J. R., & Gaskins, S. (2014). Supporting family conversations and children’s STEM learning in a children’s museum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(3), 333–344. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.004

Harlen, W. (2001). Primary Science: Taking the Plunge (2nd edition). Heinemann.

Harlen, W., & Qualter, A. (2004). The teaching of science in primary schools. London David Fulton Publishers.

Harris, P. L., Koenig, M., Corriveau, K. H., & Jaswal, V. K. (2018). Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 251–73. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710

Hickling, A. K., & Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 668–683. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.668

Hobson, S. M., Trundle, K. C., & Saçkes, M. (2010). Using a planetarium software program to promote conceptual change with young children. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19(2), 165-176. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-009-9189-8

Inan, H. Z., Trundle, K. C., & Kantor, R. (2010). Understanding natural sciences education in a Reggio Emilia-inspired preschool. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47(10), 1186–1208. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20375

Jant, E. A., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., & Babcock, E. (2014). Conversation and object manipulation influence children’s learning in a museum. Child Development, 85(5), 2029-2045. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12252

Jipson, J. L., Gülgöz, S., & Gelman, S. A. (2016). Parent–child conversations regarding the ontological status of a robotic dog. Cognitive Development, 39, 21–35. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.03.001

Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15(10), 661–667. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00737.x

Kurkul, K. E., Castine, E., Leech, K., & Corriveau, K. H. (2021). How does a switch work? The relation between adult mechanistic language and children’s learning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 72, 101221. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101221

Kurkul, K. E., & Corriveau, K. H. (2018). Question, explanation, follow-up: A mechanism for learning from others?. Child Development, 89(1), 280–294. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12726

Kurkul, K. E., Dwyer, J., & Corriveau, K. H. (2022). ‘What do YOU think?’: Children’s questions, teacher’s responses and children’s follow-up across diverse preschool settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 58, 231–241. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.09.010

Lanphear, J., & Vandermaas-Peeler, M. (2017). Inquiry and intersubjectivity in a Reggio Emilia–inspired preschool. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 31(4), 597–614. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2017.1348412

Lee, Y., & Kinzie, M. B. (2012). Teacher question and student response with regard to cognition and language use. Instructional Science, 40(6), 857–874. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-011-9193-2

Legare, C. H., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children’s causal explanatory reasoning. Child Development, 81(3), 929–944. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01443.x

Legare, C. H., & Lombrozo, T. (2014). Selective effects of explanation on learning during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 198–212. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.001

Legare, C. H., Mills, C. M., Souza, A. L., Plummer, L. E., & Yasskin, R. (2013). The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1), 63–76. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002

Legare, C. H., Sobel, D. M., & Callanan, M. (2017). Causal learning is collaborative: Examining explanation and exploration in social contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 24(5), 1548–1554. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1351-3

Lombrozo, T., Bonawitz, E. B., & Scalise, N. R. (2018). Young children’s learning and generalization of teleological and mechanistic explanations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19(2), 220–232. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2018.1427099

Leech, K. A., Haber, A. S., Jalkh, Y., & Corriveau, K. H. (2020). Embedding scientific explanations into storybooks impacts children’s scientific discourse and learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1-12. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01016

MacWhinney, B. (2000). The Childes Project: The Database, Vol. 2. London: Psychology Press.

McNeill, K. L., Berland, L. K. & Pelletier, P. (2017). Constructing explanations. In Schwarz, C., Passmore, C., Reiser, B.J. (Eds.). Helping students make sense of the world using next generation science and engineering practices (pp. 205-228). National Science Teachers Association Press.

Medina, C., & Sobel, D. M. (2020). Caregiver–child interaction influences causal learning and engagement during structured play. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 189, 104678. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104678

Mills, C. M., Danovitch, J. H., Rowles, S. P., & Campbell, I. L. (2017). Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(5), 1465–1477. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1195-2

Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Bills, M., & Mejias, C. (2010). Preschoolers use questions as a tool to acquire knowledge from different sources. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(4), 533–560. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.516419

Mills, C. M., Legare, C. H., Grant, M. G., & Landrum, A. R. (2011). Determining who to question, what to ask, and how much information to ask for: The development of inquiry in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(4), 539–560. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.003

National Research Council (1996). National science education standards. National Academies Press.

National Research Council. (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core idea. National Academic Press.

Next Generation Science Standards: For States, By States. (2013). National Academies Press.

Osborne, J., & Reigh, E. (2020). What makes a good question? Towards an epistemic classification. In L.P. Butler, S. Ronfard & K.H. Corriveau (Eds), The questioning child: Insights from psychology and education (pp. 281-330). Cambridge University Press.

Peterson, S. M., & French, L. (2008). Supporting young children's explanations through inquiry science in preschool. Early childhood research quarterly, 23(3), 395-408. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.003

Reiser, B. J., Brody, L. I. S. A., Novak, M., Tipton, K., & Adams, L. (2017). Asking questions. In C. Schwarz, C. Passmore, & B. Reiser (Eds.) Helping students make sense of the world using next generation science and engineering practices (pp. 87-108). NSTA Press, National Science Teachers Association

Ronfard, S., Zambrana, I. M., Hermansen, T. K., & Kelemen, D. (2018). Question-asking in childhood: A review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. Developmental Review, 49, 101–120. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.002

Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x

Ruggeri, A., & Lombrozo, T. (2015). Children adapt their questions to achieve efficient search. Cognition, 143, 203–216. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.004

Saçkes, M. (2013). Children’s competencies in process skills in kindergarten and their impact on academic achievement in third grade. Early Education and Development, 24(5), 704-720. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2012.715571

Saçkes, M. (2014). Parents who want their preK children to have science learning experiences are outliers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(2), 132-143. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.11.005

Saçkes, M., Flevares, L., & Trundle, K. C. (2010). Four- to six-year-old children’s conceptions of the mechanism of rainfall. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 536-546. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.01.001

Saçkes, M., Smith, M. M., & Trundle, K. C. (2016). U.S. and Turkish preschoolers’ observational knowledge of astronomy. International Journal of Science Education, 38(1), 116-129. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2015.1132858

Saçkes, M., Trundle, K. C., Bell, R. L., & O'Connell, A. A. (2011). The influence of early science experience in kindergarten on children’s immediate and later science achievement: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(2), 217-235. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20395

Saçkes, M., Trundle, K. C., & Shaheen, M. (2019). Profiling parental orientation to early childhood curriculum. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 27(5), 662-674. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2019.1651969

Saçkes, M., Trundle, K. C., & Shaheen, M. (2020). The effect of balanced learning curriculum on young children’s learning of science. Early Childhood Education Journal, 48(3), 305-312. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-019-00985-x

Sak, R. (2020). Preschoolers’ difficult questions and their teachers’ responses. Early Childhood Education Journal, 48(1), 59–70. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-019-00977-x

Skalstad, I., & Munkebye, E. (2021). Young children’s questions about science topics when situated in a natural outdoor environment: A qualitative study from kindergarten and primary school. International Journal of Science Education, 43(7), 1–19. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2021.1895451

Tabors, P. O., Roach, K. A., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Home language and literacy environment: Final results. In D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school (pp. 111–138). Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations about science: The socialization of gender inequities?. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 34–47. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34

Tizard, B., & Hughes, M. (1984). Young children learning. Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S., (1978). Mind in Society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.

Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T. A., Cullum, K. A., Letourneau, S. M., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M., & Legare, C. H. (2019). Explain this, explore that: A Study of parent–child interaction in a children’s museum. Child Development, 90(5), e598–e617. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13232

Windschitl, M., Colley, C., & Sjoberg, B. (2017). Putting it all together: Two examples of teaching with the Next Generation Science Standards. In C. Schwarz, C. Passmore, & B. Reiser (Eds.) Helping students make sense of the world using Next Generation science and engineering practices (pp. 335-352). NSTA Press, National Science Teachers Association.

Downloads

Published

2021-12-15

How to Cite

Haber, A. S., Puttre, H., Ghossainy, M. E., & Corriveau, K. H. (2021). “How will you construct a pathway system?”: Microanalysis of teacher-child scientific conversations. Journal of Childhood, Education & Society, 2(3), 338–364. https://doi.org/10.37291/2717638X.202123117

Funding data